研究者業績

大貫 良幸

Yoshiyuki Onuki

基本情報

所属
自治医科大学 医学部 脳神経外科学講座 講師
学位
Ph.D.(Erasmus University Rotterdam)

J-GLOBAL ID
201801010399817254
researchmap会員ID
B000307861

研究キーワード

 5

論文

 16
  • Yuta Inagawa, Kazumi Kurata, Seiji Obi, Yoshiyuki Onuki, Yukifumi Monden, Koyuru Kurane, Rieko Furukawa, Tadahiro Mitani, Hirotaka Nakamura, Shiro Suda, Tatsuhiko Yagihashi
    Journal of eating disorders 13(1) 86-86 2025年5月19日  
    OBJECTIVE: This study aimed to evaluate the dynamics of the neutrophil-to-lymphocyte ratio (NLR) during the initial hospitalization of patients with eating disorders (EDs) and to assess its potential as a biomarker for monitoring disease severity and treatment response. METHODS: A retrospective chart review was conducted with 55 patients aged ≤ 16 years diagnosed with anorexia nervosa or avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder and admitted to Jichi Medical University Hospital between 2015 and 2021. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics including sex, age, rate of weight gain, percentage of ideal body weight (%IBW), tube feeding treatment, and NLR were obtained. Statistical analyses used a mixed model for repeated measures to assess NLR changes regarding %IBW and other clinical factors. RESULTS: The NLR at admission was lower in the malnourished state but increased with weight recovery. MMRM revealed that tube feeding treatment (β = 0.538) and restoration of %IBW (β = 0.029) significantly predicted an increase in the NLR. The interaction between tube feeding and the quadratic term of %IBW was also significant, indicating distinct patterns of NLR changes: without tube feeding, NLR increased linearly with weight recovery, whereas with tube feeding, NLR exhibited a non-linear, upward-convex parabolic trend. DISCUSSION: These findings suggest that NLR may offer an objective recovery marker less influenced by patient self-report. Monitoring NLR before and after tube feeding may help distinguish true physiological recovery from transient stress responses, providing complementary information to conventional assessments. Further research is warranted to establish its clinical relevance.
  • Kosuke Matsuzono, Yoshiyuki Onuki, Kyoko Otsuka, Honoka Hiki, Yuhei Anan, Takafumi Mashiko, Reiji Koide, Naoto Kunii, Kensuke Kawai, Shigeru Fujimoto
    Science progress 108(1) 368504251322083-368504251322083 2025年  
    Delusional misidentification, a rare syndrome in which a patient displays persistent delusional misidentification of individuals or objects, occurs in several types of dementia. However, the pathology of delusional misidentification is still unclear, and there was no data pertaining to striate-frontal projection. Here, we report a case of delusional misidentification following frontotemporal dementia in which complex striate-frontal and some specific frontal gyrus dysfunction were observed. In our presented case, delusional misidentification progressed following frontal atrophy. Believing that her actual daughter had been replaced by her niece, her symptoms of delusional misidentification and frontal atrophy progressed in the short term, and social arrangement was necessary three months after the onset. There were no abnormal neurological findings including parkinsonism and general cognitive function test scores were preserved. Validated by dopamine transporter single-photon emission computed tomography, right unilateral striatal uptake decreased significantly without parkinsonism or Parkinson's disease. In addition, of specific concern, functional magnetic resonance images showed left opercular inferior frontal gyrus and right superior frontal gyrus dysfunctions. Our case study highlights complex striate-frontal projection and specific frontal gyrus dysfunctions associated with the pathology of delusional misidentification syndrome.
  • Chaoyi Qin, Frederic Michon, Yoshiyuki Onuki, Yohei Ishishita, Keisuke Otani, Kensuke Kawai, Pascal Fries, Valeria Gazzola, Christian Keysers
    Cell reports 42(11) 113432-113432 2023年11月13日  
    The action observation network (AON) has been extensively studied using short, isolated motor acts. How activity in the network is altered when these isolated acts are embedded in meaningful sequences of actions remains poorly understood. Here we utilized intracranial electrocorticography to characterize how the exchange of information across key nodes of the AON-the precentral, supramarginal, and visual cortices-is affected by such embedding and the resulting predictability. We found more top-down beta oscillation from precentral to supramarginal contacts during the observation of predictable actions in meaningful sequences compared to the same actions in randomized, and hence less predictable, order. In addition, we find that expectations enabled by the embedding lead to a suppression of bottom-up visual responses in the high-gamma range in visual areas. These results, in line with predictive coding, inform how nodes of the AON integrate information to process the actions of others.
  • Shinichi Kumagai, Tomoyo Isoguchi Shiramatsu, Akane Matsumura, Yohei Ishishita, Kenji Ibayashi, Yoshiyuki Onuki, Kensuke Kawai, Hirokazu Takahashi
    Brain Stimulation 16(5) 1476-1485 2023年9月  
  • Efe Soyman, Rune Bruls, Kalliopi Ioumpa, Laura Müller-Pinzler, Selene Gallo, Chaoyi Qin, Elisabeth C W van Straaten, Matthew W Self, Judith C Peters, Jessy K Possel, Yoshiyuki Onuki, Johannes C Baayen, Sander Idema, Christian Keysers, Valeria Gazzola
    eLife 11 2022年11月3日  
    Based on neuroimaging data, the insula is considered important for people to empathize with the pain of others. Here we present intracranial electroencephalographic (iEEG) recordings and single-cell recordings from the human insulae while 7 epilepsy patients rated the intensity of a woman's painful experiences seen in short movie clips. Pain had to be deduced from seeing facial expressions or a hand being slapped by a belt. We found activity in the broadband 20-190 Hz range correlated with the trial-by-trial perceived intensity in the insula for both types of stimuli. Within the insula, some locations had activity correlating with perceived intensity for our facial expressions but not for our hand stimuli, others only for our hand but not our face stimuli, and others for both. The timing of responses to the sight of the hand being hit is best explained by kinematic information; that for our facial expressions, by shape information. Comparing the broadband activity in the iEEG signal with spiking activity from a small number of neurons and an fMRI experiment with similar stimuli, revealed a consistent spatial organization, with stronger associations with intensity more anteriorly, while viewing the hand being slapped.

MISC

 28

共同研究・競争的資金等の研究課題

 7